When a worm avoids blue — it's a breakthrough

Worm Wonders

An eyeless worm has upended scientific understanding of color

When a worm avoids blue — it's a breakthrough.

Updated: 
Originally Published: 

The microscopic roundworm, Caenorhabditis elegans, might be Earth's secret superhero. These tube-shaped creatures, which thrive on rotting vegetation, may seem unremarkable at first glance. But this simple animal has benefited humanity in numerous ways.

NASA scientists are sending C. elegans to space to test the effects of muscle mass loss. They are categorized as the ideal "model organism" to study the development of disease and aging. At just 1 millimeter long, its transparent body allows scientists to examine fundamental cellular and molecular processes.

But scientists may have just unveiled another one of this organism's superpowers.

Caenorhabditis elegans worm in a colored scanning electron micrograph (SEM).

Getty

What's new — A study published Thursday in the journal Science finds C. elegans can discriminate between colors despite the fact that they don't have eyes or opsins — a colorless protein that reacts to light. The research was conducted by scientists at Yale University and the Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

Dipon Ghosh, the corresponding author of the study and a postdoctoral researcher at MIT, tells Inverse these findings suggests the "worms were sensitive to the color of light in their environment."

How they did it — The researchers tested how these roundworms avoid a certain blue toxic pigment known as pyocyanin by using white light.

The researchers first tested whether a harmless blue dye, in combination with white light, would cause the roundworm to avoid the synthetic substance — similar to how it used white light to avoid the blue pyocanin.

"We wanted to understand which component of the blue pigment toxin pyocyanin – its color or its toxicity – was informing the worms’ avoidance," Ghosh says.

"We could reproduce the effects of pyocyanin on bacterial avoidance with both a harmless blue dye and a clear, colorless toxin, but not either separately."

The scientists wanted to figure out the meaning behind the roundworm's biological responses. The difference in the roundworm's response to the colorless toxin and the harmless blue dye gave the researchers the necessary information to continue their experiment.

"This result told us that the worms were assessing both the toxic nature and the color of pyocyanin when deciding to avoid their food," Ghosh says.

"This result told us that worms were sensitive to the color of light in their environment."

The second step in the experiment involved modifying the white light to create a simulation of the toxic bacteria. Would the worms still avoid the simulated pyocyanin under these conditions?

"We wanted to see whether we could 'simulate' bacterial pigmentation by simply changing the color of the light. We got a blue filter that changes the color of the white light that we were using to a color more similar to the blue color of the pigment," Ghosh says.

They exposed one set of bacteria to light filtered by color, and another to non-filtered light. The worms that received exposure to filtered light avoided the bacteria, but the other set of worms stayed on the bacteria.

An aquatic nematode. C. elegans fit into the broader class of nematodes, though they are soil dwellers.

Getty

"This result told us that worms were sensitive to the color of light in their environment," Ghosh says.

Finally, the researchers used two LED lights to heighten the stakes and confirm their findings: one blue and one amber. Amber was used in an attempt to replicate environmental light conditions.

"We found that even if we exposed the worms to the same total energy, worms avoided some blue-amber color mixes but not others," Ghosh says.

This final step enabled researchers to confirm that roundworms can accurately discriminate between different colors, in turn helping them stay alive.

Ultimately, the team discovered C. elegans aren't simply evaluating levels of brightness — they're comparing the "rations of wavelengths," Ghosh explains. They have the ability to discriminate between colors and use that information to make decisions, all without the benefit of eyes.

Digging into the details — Overall, the researchers discovered roundworms can discriminate between different colors, using white light to move away from blue-colored toxic bacteria.

This finding upends conventional scientific wisdom, which dictates organisms must possess the photoreceptor genes in opsins to discriminate between colors. C. elegans, remarkably, was able to discriminate between the colors despite lacking opsins.

The scientists also identified two sets of genes — chtl-1 and jkk-1 as well as F52H3.6 and lec-3 — that contribute to the roundworm's avoidant responses to color, setting up the potential for future genetic research.

A figure from the study showing how the roundworm discriminates between colors to avoid danger.

Dipon Ghosh et. al.

Why it matters — This roundworm's color discrimination is no mere quirk, but an evolutionary gift, the researchers suggest.

"Our work also suggests that sensitivity to specific colors could offer some evolutionary benefit for the worm as it navigates an environment where it might encounter colorful microbes," Ghosh says.

But Ghosh also believes the findings of the research go beyond roundworms, reshaping the way scientists think about the evolution of color discrimination. The work suggests opsins are not necessary for an animal to tell the difference between colors, for example. If opsins aren't actually necessary to perceive colors, then we might need to expand how we define color sensitivity in other animals.

"Because it seems that worms have co-opted evolutionarily conserved genes for color sensitivity, we think our findings could generalize to other animals or even tissues that sense light, even if they don’t possess the classic molecules required for vision per se," Ghosh says.

Microbial pigments might affect color perception in these other animals in a similar way to the roundworm, he adds.

"Perhaps microbial pigmentation can more generally affect foraging by wandering hosts other than C. elegans – meaning that colors might be involved in unexpected ways in other interspecies interactions as well," Ghosh says.

C. elegans undergo examination by project scientists at the Kennedy Space Center in Florida in 2003.

Getty

What's next — The study raises exciting questions for future research.

Specifically, the researchers would like to better understand the link between different genes and color sensitivity.

"Because we know that many genes are contributing to color sensitivity in addition to the two we've already identified, I'd like to determine what other genes contribute to color sensitivity," Ghosh says.

Based on their current paper, these other genes could also possibly aid in foraging for food. Ultimately, this ongoing research will provide the stepping stone to understanding the evolution of color sensitivity in all living creatures.

"By better understanding color sensitivity in the worm, given that the genes involved could be evolutionarily conserved, we might find similar patterns and activities in other organisms," Ghosh says.

Abstract: Color detection is used by animals of diverse phyla to navigate colorful natural environments and is thought to require evolutionarily conserved opsin photoreceptor genes. We report that Caenorhabditis elegans roundworms can discriminate between colors despite the fact that they lack eyes and opsins. Specifically, we found that white light guides C. elegans foraging decisions away from a blue-pigment toxin secreted by harmful bacteria. These foraging decisions are guided by specific blue-to-amber ratios of light. The color specificity of color-dependent foraging varies notably among wild C. elegans strains, which indicates that color discrimination is ecologically important. We identified two evolutionarily conserved cellular stress response genes required for opsin-independent, color-dependent foraging byC. elegans, and we speculate that cellular stress response pathways can mediate spectral discrimination by photosensitive cells and organisms—even by those lacking opsins.

This article was originally published on

Related Tags