Intelligent Life

This surprising cephalopod challenges our understanding of human evolution

New research shines a light on the origins of intelligence.

Updated: 
Originally Published: 
Giphy

Consider the cuttlefish. According to new research, this cephalopod has a trick hidden up its tentacles — or rather, its large brain. It reveals one surprising aspect of this creature's intelligence: self-control.

These findings may inform an ongoing scientific endeavor: understanding the evolution of intelligence.

What's new — This marine mollusk, also known as Sepia officinalis, may (debatably) not be the cutest cephalopod in town, but it's by far one of the most intriguing. Most famously, the cuttlefish can camouflage within its ocean surroundings, hiding from the unwanted attention of predators.

A study published Tuesday in the journal Proceedings of the Royal Society finds the cuttlefish can also demonstrate self-control by delaying gratification in order to snare higher-quality prey.

Lead author Alexandra Schnell, a researcher at the University of Cambridge, tells Inverse this finding was unexpected.

"Finding evidence for self-control in a cuttlefish, an invertebrate that diverged from the vertebrate lineage over 550 million years ago, is surprising," Schnell says.

A cuttlefish in an experimental aquarium.

Alex Schnell

Some background — In the late 1960s and early 1970s, researcher Walter Mischel conducted the now-famous Stanford "marshmallow experiment."

In the marshmallow test, Mischel tested pre-school-aged children's willpower through a simple experiment: Researchers placed a marshmallow in front of the child. If the child could resist the temptation to consume the marshmallow for 15 minutes, they would get a second sweet treat. But if the child gave in too early, they got just the one treat.

The Stanford research highlighted the connections between self-control and certain cognitive skills. The researchers in this cuttlefish study drew upon this experiment for inspiration.

"We adapted the Stanford marshmallow test so that it was more cuttlefish 'friendly," Schnell says.

Alex Schnell examines the cuttlefish.

Grass Foundation

How they did it — Researchers trained, then tested cuttlefish in an experimental aquarium using two methods designed to evaluate whether the animal could learn to resist the short-term temptation to nibble on lower-quality food and ultimately hold out for higher-quality food.

"This involved first testing the food preferences of each individual cuttlefish that was involved in the study and then using their first preference as the delayed reward and their second preference as the tempting immediate reward," Schnell says.

Instead of marshmallows, the preferred treat was some live grass shrimp. The less preferred choice was a bit of raw king prawn.

The team also tested how long the cephalopods were willing to resist temptation by starting with a delay of 10 seconds. The delay was subsequently increased in 10-second time intervals.

The researchers came away with two main findings:

  • Cuttlefish could delay gratification to snag a higher-quality prey
  • They were able to delay for a considerable period of time, ranging from 50 to 130 seconds

Why it matters — The new findings suggest a connection between self-control and learning in non-primate animals for the very first time.

The cuttlefish "showed comparable capacities for self-control to what we see in apes, corvids, and parrots," Schnell says.

Previous research on the development of willpower — and its association with learning and intelligence — have mostly focused on humans and primates like chimps.

The reasons for the evolution of self-control in primates and humans are more straightforward, according to Schnell. These include delaying hunting so you can build tools for the future, strengthening social bonds, and living a longer life.

An illustration of the testing conditions from the study.

Schnell et. al.

"However, apes, corvids, and parrots are long-lived and social and thus the benefits for self-control are obvious for these animals," Schnell explains.

The evolution of self-control is not so clear in an invertebrate like the cuttlefish, which typically doesn't live to see its third birthday.

"Consequently, our understanding of why self-control evolved has always been based on evolutionary pressures that are relevant to long-lived social species," Schnell says.

Unlike non-human primates, the study speculates that cephalopods may have evolved self-control for a different reason: their eating habits.

The cuttlefish goes for its treat.

Grass Foundation

Cuttlefish often remain camouflaged, hidden in one place for a long time. They have limited and sporadic opportunities to hunt for prey and forage in the wild. The 'sit and wait' nature of the cuttlefish may have increased its capacity for self-control.

"The ability to exert self-control might increase their foraging success because ambush attacks are likely to be more successful when cuttlefish are within close proximity to their prey," researchers write in the study.

By establishing the capacity for self-control in cuttlefish, researchers are one step closer to unlocking the evolutionary origins of intelligence.

"Identifying comparable cognitive abilities across disparate animal lineages provides a more comprehensive understanding of the origins of intelligence," Schnell says.

What's next — While the results of the experiment are promising, we have a long way to go before we fully understand the evolution of self-control in animals — especially invertebrates like the cuttlefish.

Specifically, we don't understand the behavioral mechanisms driving these behaviors. Why do cephalopods demonstrate self-control? And what does this capacity for self-control tell us about the evolution of intelligence in living beings?

These questions remain unanswered for now. Schnell says that this lack of knowledge stems from the limited, "largely neglected" research on self-control and other cognitive functions in invertebrate species.

Octopuses could serve as an appropriate subject for future research due to their unique foraging habits, in which "they move forward pause briefly, observe their surroundings, then typically change direction," the study team writes.

"Efforts to expand the range of models species will help locate the origins of cognitive traits and also help us obtain a more comprehensive understanding of the evolution of intelligence," Schnell says.

Abstract: The ability to exert self-control varies within and across taxa. Some species can exert self-control for several seconds whereas others, such as large-brained vertebrates, can tolerate delays of up to several minutes. Advanced self-control has been linked to better performance in cognitive tasks and has been hypothesized to evolve in response to specific socio-ecological press-ures. These pressures are difficult to uncouple because previously studied species face similar socio-ecological challenges. Here, we investigate self-control and learning performance in cuttlefish, an invertebrate that is thought to have evolved under partially different pressures to previously studied vertebrates. To test self-control, cuttlefish were presented with a delay maintenance task, which measures an individual’s ability to forgo immediate gratification and sustain a delay for a better but delayed reward. Cuttlefish maintained delay durations for up to 50–130 s. To test learning performance, we used a reversal-learning task, whereby cuttlefish were required to learn to associate the reward with one of two stimuli and then subsequently learn to associate the reward with the alternative stimulus. Cuttlefish that delayed gratification for longer had better learning performance. Our results demonstrate that cuttlefish can tolerate delays to obtain the food of higher quality comparable to that of some large-brained vertebrates.

This article was originally published on

Related Tags